Several articles ago, I took a look at the possibility that knuckleball pitchers might exert some “hangover” effect on opposing batters extending beyond their own appearances on the mound. The notion that facing the knuckleball might possess some potential to “mess up” a batter’s swing has long been a part of baseball lore, causing players and managers alike to ascribe post-knuckleball slumps to the mysterious force of the flutterball. But could the rumors of a knuckleball hangover hold up on a baseball edition of “Mythbusters”?
In part one of this two-part series, my cursory initial review of performance following exposure to Tim Wakefield seemed to reveal some sort of lingering penalty, but I mentioned that I’d like to subject the knuckleball hangover to a more rigorous investigation, controlling for additional factors in an attempt to determine whether the effect was real. Readers expressed interest in a follow-up, and with the help of our newest full-timer, Colin Wyers, I’ve managed to settle the matter (for the most part).
Here’s what I found last time for the performance of opposing batters in games following starts by Wakefield:
Time Period |
SOr |
UBBr |
HRr |
|||||
1 Day After |
11105 |
.265 |
.332 |
.416 |
.748 |
.168 |
.080 |
.025 |
2 Days After |
10460 |
.271 |
.338 |
.429 |
.767 |
.168 |
.078 |
.028 |
3 Days After |
10274 |
.280 |
.345 |
.435 |
.781 |
.163 |
.078 |
.030 |
Seasonal |
|
.274 |
.344 |
.440 |
.784 |
.159 |
.083 |
.030 |
With the old methodology, there seemed to be something to the notion that Wakefield’s floater impaired opposing batters for at least a day or two after his starts. However, as we’re about to see, our new methodology isn’t quite so sanguine about his ability to reach beyond the box score in an effort to suppress offense. This time around, we used “new” TAv instead of OPS, which allowed for a more accurate assessment of offensive performance. In addition, we controlled for the prowess of the pitchers following Wakefield, as well as each batter’s seasonal performance, using the Log5 method, which gave us an expected TAv for each batter on each day removed from a confrontation with Wakefield.
Time Period |
Expected |
Actual |
Same Game |
.261 |
.260 |
1 Game After |
.264 |
.258 |
2 Games After |
.265 |
.270 |
3 Games After |
.268 |
.267 |
That “Same Day” figure includes both plate appearances against Wakefield and plate appearances against the relievers who followed Wakefield in his starts; isolating the performance against the relievers alone would be ideal, but were a sizeable effect out there, it would likely still show up in the cumulative figure. Batters did perform slightly worse than expected in their first game after facing Wakefield, but exhibited no handicap in either of the following two games. If there is an effect, it doesn’t seem to be one of a magnitude large enough for the Red Sox to go out of their way to exploit it, which they could consider doing by manipulating their rotation in order to start Wakefield more often in the first game of a series, or choosing to pitch him in the first game of a doubleheader wherever possible (neither of which they appear to have done with any regularity over the course of his career).
“New” TAv has not yet been park-adjusted, so it’s possible, if unlikely, that a hangover effect associated with Wakefield’s knuckler could have been disguised above by the fact that a substantial portion of the games following his starts took place in Fenway Park, a favorable hitter’s environment. With the aid of The Neyer/James Guide to Pitchers and several other sources, I compiled a list of starters who featured the knuckleball and were active at any point from 1972 on (the period during which Retrosheet’s records are complete enough for a study of this nature). In cases where I could establish that a pitcher adopted the knuckleball mid-career, any data from before the pitch became a critical component of the converted knuckleballer’s repertoire was dismissed. That left me with the following list:
Name |
Usage Notes |
Post-1984 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Post-1971 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Tim Wakefield |
|
|
So, what happens when we run the numbers for that list of 12 knuckleballers, a sample consisting of almost 32,000 data points?
Time Period |
Expected |
Actual |
Same Game |
.257 |
.259 |
1 Game After |
.263 |
.263 |
2 Games After |
.262 |
.266 |
3 Games After |
.262 |
.267 |
blaming an entire road trip’s worth of offensive ineptitude on one’s offerings seems like quite a stretch.
Those may not be the exciting findings we were hoping to generate, but the results seem fairly conclusive: batters need not consider consuming a draft of knuckleball-flavored hair of the dog after facing the flutterball. In short, it’s possible that the knuckleball hangover effect exists for certain batters, or even for certain pitchers, but the evidence suggests that it’s neither a major factor, nor an inherent trait of knuckleballs on the whole. The knuckleball may be a nuisance to hit, but its widespread reputation for fouling up swings is likely the result of confirmation bias and selective memory. Teams may not be pleased to see an opposing knuckleballer on the mound, butThank you for reading
This is a free article. If you enjoyed it, consider subscribing to Baseball Prospectus. Subscriptions support ongoing public baseball research and analysis in an increasingly proprietary environment.
Subscribe now